Description:
Readings this week in EDAT 6115 explored
different views on when and how children develop cognitively, how language and
literacy develop, and the implications of developmental stages in education.
The research and findings of Vygotski, Piaget, and Bronfenbrenner were also
examined.
Piaget’s constructivist
theory views cognitive development “as a process in which children actively build
systems of meaning and understandings of reality through their experiences and
interactions” (Slavin, 2018, p.26). He defined
stages of cognitive development (sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete
operational, and formal operational) that children progress through, providing
benchmarks for cognitive growth often used by physicians, psychologists, and educators
(Slavin, 2018, p. 26). Piaget’s thoughts
on cognitive development are deeply embedded in curriculum and instruction
throughout education. Piaget’s theories have shaped our “developmentally
appropriate education”
structure and encouraged educators to focus on 1) how children think not just
what they can do 2) creating inquiry-based and experiential learning environments
3) age-appropriate cognition and 4) differentiation for learning differences
(Slavin, 2018, p. 32-33). Piaget’s stages
of cognitive development have historically been used to establish curriculum
and performance expectations for students within the classroom. For example, the adolescents in my classroom
(aged 14-18) should easily be able to think abstractly and solve problems through
experimentation since they have entered the formal operational stage (Slavin,
2018, p.27). This task would be too
advanced cognitively for students in earlier stages of Piaget’s framework who may
lack the ability to think logically or abstractly. Criticisms of Piaget’s theories
center around the idea that “development precedes learning” and that children develop
in predictable, age-based stages progressively (Slavin, 2018, p.31). Research has shown that when taught or exposed
to higher level tasks, children often respond by being able to complete tasks
in higher Piagetian stages (Slavin, 20188, p.32).
Vygotsky’s
theories about cognitive development focused more on the environmental context of
learning. He proposed that cognitive development was more of an “outgrowth of
social development through interaction with others and the environment” (Slavin,
2018, p. 42). Vygotsky’s stages of
development rely on the increasing ability of children to be self-regulate their
response to signs in their environment. The most impactful of Vygotsky’s thoughts
is his explanation of the zone of proximal development. Tasks within this zone
are those that “a child cannot yet accomplish alone but could accomplish with
that assistance of more competent peers or adults” (Slavin, 2018, p.34). Vygotsky’s
influence on instructional strategies such as cooperative learning,
scaffolding, and ability grouping. Like Vygotsky, Bronfenbrenner linked cognitive
development to the influence of environmental factors but expanded his focus to
include all social and institutional influences. Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological
view of cognitive development emphasizes the “interconnectedness of the many
factors that influence a child’s development” including the sociocultural factors
that show “a more complete model of the influences beyond biology” (Slavin, 2018, p.36).
The understanding
of how children cognitively develop is essential to educators, particularly those
teaching language and literacy at young ages.
Whether biological or environmental, influences on cognitive development
are important for teachers to understand and plan intentionally for. Language and literacy are the cornerstones for
further education as learning all other subjects requires a strong foundation
in reading and comprehension. Creating environments in school and at home that
emphasize the importance of language and literacy are critical for cognitive development
and further academic success (Slavin, 2018, p. 38).
My own classroom
is typically composed of high achieving, mostly gifted students ranging in age
from 14 to 18. As a Biologist and educator, I am no stranger to the nature
versus nurture argument that often arises when discussing cognitive
development. I have seen just about every scenario in my 17 years in the
classroom. I have had students that are cognitively further advanced at 14 than
most are at 18. I have had students with ridiculous cognitive potential that
cannot be successful because of environmental influences beyond their control. Regardless
of what factors are shaping students’ cognitive development, it is our responsibility
as educators to not only be able to identify where our students are but to also
be able to differentiate for them to be successful. Identifying a students’ zone
of proximal development, understanding their sociocultural influences, and intentionally
differentiating for their cognitive abilities is all a part of being an
effective educator. For me, understanding what tasks my students should be
capable of doing allows me to prepare lessons that are engaging and challenging. My goal is to always create lessons that provide
the right balance of productive struggle for students and encourage a growth mindset.
Reference
Slavin, R.E. (2018). Educational Psychology: Theory and
Practice (12th ed.). Pearson.
Great job on this first blog posting. Please know that if you keep journaling at this level, you will be in fine standing when it comes time to complete the final course project. Well written and well done!
ReplyDelete